Biographie

Werner-Annette
Annette Werner heads the Colour and Vision Lab http://www.annettewerner.com at the Department for Ophthalmoloy of the Eberhard Karls University Tübingen. She studied biology at the Free University Berlin, and received a diploma in neurobiology and zoology. She completed her Ph.D. at the laboratory of Prof. R. Menzel, Institute of Neurobiology, Free University Berlin, where she examined colour vision in honeybees (Thesis: Colour Constancy in the Honeybee, Apis mellifera; 1990)

As a postdoc she has worked at laboratories in Munich, Freiburg, Hongkong, and New York, covering clinical and basic research on human vision. Annette Werner relocated to Tübingen in 2002, where she buildt up the Colour and Vision Group at the Institute for Ophthalmic Research; she is also a member of the Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience (CIN) http://www.cin.uni-tuebingen.de. Annette Werner studies and teaches human and animal color perception, her present research interests focus on the effects of gentherapy in achromatopsia, the neural computations underlying colour constancy, as well as the origin of perceptual ambiguities (#TheDress) and the development of intercultural colour preferences.

Abstract: The Real Colour of #The Dress

What is the real colour of #The Dress? Few topics in colour research have generated - in science community and public alike - as much interest, as this phenomenon; it refers to the observation that observers cannot agree on the percieved colour of a dress seen in a particular photograph, i.e. colour judgements can be as extreme as blue&black or white&gold. Although individual differences in visual performances are well known, this phenomenon is still surprising because it uncovers an unprecendented ambiguity in colour perception. Understanding the origin of the perceptual dichotomy of #TheDress is therefore important not only in the context of the phenomenon itself but also for our comprehension of the neural computations of colour in general. I will review several hypotheses put forward to explain #The Dress phenomenon, ranging from individual differences in the eyes’ optics to high end, cognitive epxlanations, and show to audiance some surprising manipulations of the dress’ appearance.


Die Wahren Farben des Kleides

Welche Farbe hat das Kleid von #The Dress wirklich? Selten hat eine Frage die Wissenschaft und die Öffentlichkeit gleichermaßen elektrisiert. Das Phänomen bezieht sich auf das Aussehen eines Kleides, dessen Farbe auf einem bestimmten Foto von verschiedenen Beobachtern extrem unterschiedlich wahrgenommen wird – der Farbeindruck variiert dabei zwischen Blau & Schwarz bis nach Weiß & Gold. Unterschiede in individuellen Sehleistungen sind seit langem bekannt, aber das #The Dress Phänomen it dennoch überraschend, da es eine bisher nicht bekannte Mehrdeutigkeit in der Farbwahrnehmung aufzeigt. Es ist daher von erheblicher Bedeutung, den Ursprung dieser Mehrdeutigkeit zu aufzudecken, sowohl für das Verständnis des Phänomens selbst wie auch für das Verstehen der neuronalen Verarbeitungsschritte beim Enstehen von Farbahrnehmung im allgemeinen. In meinem Vortrag werde ich die wichtigsten Hypothesen zu dieser Problematik zusammenfassen, angefangen bei individuellen Unterschieden des menschlichen Auges bis hin zu Erklärungen durch kognitive Prozesse auf kortikaler Ebene; die Zuhörer – und Seher können außerdem mitverfolgen, wie die Farbwahrnehmung im Falle des #The Dress durch Manipulationen von Beleuchtung und Kontext des Kleides variiert werden kann – nach Blau & Schwarz oder Weiß & Gold.



Biographie

KarenSchloss
Karen Schloss is an Assistant Professor at the University of Wisconsin – Madison in the Department of Psychology and Wisconsin Institute for Discovery – Living Environments Lab. She received her BA from Barnard College, Columbia University in 2005, with a major in Psychology and a minor in Architecture. She then conducted her graduate studies with Stephen Palmer at the University of California, Berkeley, where she received her PhD in Psychology in 2011. She continued working with Stephen Palmer as a postdoctoral scholar from 2011-2013. In 2013, Karen became an Assistant Professor of Research at Brown University in the Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences.
At Brown, she formed her
Visual Perception and Cognition Lab, which investigates how colors influence the way people think, feel, and behave. Her research focuses on what determines people's aesthetic preferences for colors, how those preferences influence decision making, and how colors can be used to communicate meaning in information visualizations. Karen recently moved her lab to the University of Wisconsin – Madison (Summer 2016), where she will extend her research to understand how colors influence judgments and behavior in immersive virtual environments.

Abstract: Color-Emotion Associations: Not so Hue-Based as Previously Thought

Colors are powerful tools for communicating emotion. In film, art, and design, colors signal a wide variety of emotions, including happiness, sadness, and anger. Traditionally, the emotional associations of colors are discussed in terms of hue: e.g., yellow is happy, blue is sad, and red is angry. However, we present evidence that this hue-based account can be misleading and even erroneous, and argue that it has stood in the way of answering deeper questions concerning how color-emotion associations are formed and why they exist. The present study set out to determine whether there is a hue-based component to the happiness vs. sadness of colors, after controlling for chroma and lightness. It was motivated by two key components. First, color-emotion spaces (e.g., as determined through multidimensional scaling) are primarily structured according to the dimensions of saturation and lightness. Second, the most saturated (and most prototypical) yellow is brighter than the most saturated (and most prototypical) blue. In an effort to test saturated colors, studies on color emotion associations have typically confounded hue and lightness by testing lighter yellows and darker blues. Has yellow been shown to be happier than blue, merely because it was lighter? We addressed this question by having participants rate the happiness vs. sadness of 32 colors (8 hues x 2 lightness (L*) levels x 2 chroma (c*) levels).

Lightness dominated their judgments (82% variance explained). Within light colors, happiness was not modulated by yellowness-blueness, but within dark saturated colors, bluer colors were actually happier than yellower colors. A follow-up experiment using colors that were perceptually matched for saturation showed the same pattern. The results imply that in the pursuit of understanding color-happiness associations, we should ask why lighter color are happier, and asking why yellower colors are happier has been the wrong question.



Biographie

almutkelber
Studium der Biologie an den Universitäten von Mainz, Sussex und Tübingen, Promotion in Tübingen über die Orientierung von Bienen. Nach Aufenthalten in Brasilien und Australien seit 1998 an der Universität Lund, da seit 2007 Professor für Sinnesbiologie. Innerhalb dieses Feldes untersucht Almut Kelber seit Jahren das Farbensehen verschiedener Tiere, darunter Schmetterlinge, Bienen, Vögel, Pferde und Seehunde. Besonderer Fokus liegt auf der Suche nach den Begrenzungen und den Anpassungen des Sehsystems an das Verhalten einer Art sowie and verschiendene ökologische Faktoren wie etwas die Lichtverhältnisse.

Abstract: Colour Vision in dim Light

In bright sunlight, we see the world in sparkling colours, but when night falls, colours fade away until, with less than a half moon, everything appears in 50 shades of grey. The reason for this lies in our duplex retina with three types of cones - noisy photoreceptors - and only one type of rods - receptors optimized for low noise. Colour discrimination is based on comparison of cone signals, and limited by receptor noise. Over a large intensity range, the level of receptor noise stays constant, but in dim light, photon shot noise statrts impairing colour discrimination, and the absolute threshold of cone-based vision is set by dark noise in cones. Like humans, other vertebrates with duplex retinae use colour-blind rod vision in dim light.
Nocturnal insects and vertebrates lacking rods have to use the same set of receptors day and night. They have evolved adaptations to increase the sensitivity of their eyes and photoreceptors, and to reduce receptor noise. This allows them to use chromatic vision in dimmer light intensities than humans. Frogs and toads that have two types of rod use opponent signals from rods to control phototaxis even at their visual threshold, at light intensities ten times darker than the absolute threshold of human vision. However, for tasks such as prey or mate choice, their colour discrimination abilities fail at brighter light intensities, only ten times darker than the humans colour vision threshold, limited by the dark noise in their cones.
In this presentation, I review what is known about dim-light colour vision and compare colour vision thresholds with the optical sensitivity of the photoreceptors in selected animal species with lens and compound eyes.



Biographie Prof. Dr. Susanne Marschall

Susanne Marschall
Susanne Marschall hat den Lehrstuhl für Film- und Fernsehwissenschaft am Institut für Medienwissenschaft der Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen inne. Sie ist Direktorin des dortigen Zentrums für Medienkompetenz sowie Sprecherin des neu gegründeten Tübinger Forschungszentrums für Animation. Sie studierte Deutsche Philologie, Komparatistik und Philosophie in Köln und Mainz und promovierte als Stipendiatin im DFG-Graduiertenkolleg „Drama und Theater als Paradigma der Moderne“ über Tanz und Körperdiskurse im Drama der Jahrhundertwende (TextTanzTheater, 1996). Sie lehrte und forschte nach der Promotion am Seminar für Filmwissenschaft an der Johannes Gutenberg-Universität in Mainz und habilitierte sich dort mit einer Forschungsarbeit zur Bildästhetik des Kinos (Farbe im Kino, 1. Aufl., 2005, 2. Aufl. 2009). 2003 erhielt sie den Lehrpreis für exzellente Leistungen in der Lehre, 2012 wurde sie von der Zeitschrift Unicum zur Professorin des Jahres 2012 gewählt. Zahlreiche Veröffentlichungen zu Farbe, Licht und Bildkomposition im Film, zur Bildsymbolik im interkulturellen Vergleich, zum indischen Kino, zu TV Serien, Tanzgeschichte und Schauspielkunst, Dokumentarfilm, Wahrnehmungstheorie und Emotionsforschung, Mythentheorie und moderner Poetik. Autorin (gemeinsam mit R. Bieberstein und K. Schneider) der Filmdokumentation Lotte Reiniger – Tanz der Schatten (2012, Universität Tübingen, EIKON Südwest, arte). Ihre aktuell zentralen Forschungsschwerpunkte sind Licht, Farbe und Bildgestaltung im Film, die indische Film- und Medienkultur sowie das weite Feld der Animation.

Biographie Prof. Dr. Achim Mittag

mittag_ri_01

Achim Mittag, geb. 1958; 1988/89 Promotion bei H. Schmidt-Glintzer in München; 1986-1994 Wissenschaftlicher Assistent; 1994-2005 Wissenschaftlicher Mitarbeiter, Lehrtätigkeit und Professurvertretung in Bielefeld, Leiden, Marburg, New York, Essen. Seit 2005 Professor für Chinesische Sprache, Literatur und Philosophie an der Universität Tübingen; Forschungsschwerpunkt: Chinesische Historiographie und historisches Denken in China. Wichtige Publikationen: Historical Truth, Historical Criticism, and Ideology. Chinese Historiography and Historical Culture from a New Comparative Perspective (Hrsg., mit H. Schmidt-Glintzer u. J. Rüsen; 2005); Conceiving the "Empire". China and Rome Compared (Hrsg., mit F.-H. Mutschler; 2008).

Abstract: Love, Passion and All-Under-Heaven: Die Ordnung der Farben in Zhang Yimous Hero

Der namenslose Held in Zhang Yimous Hero (2003) trägt Schwarz, während er dem Kaiser die Vorgeschichte eines geplanten Attentats erzählt, das von ihm schließlich doch nicht ausgeführt werden wird. Wie in dem japanischen Film Rashomon von Akira Kurosawa aus dem Jahr 1950 fächert sich diese Vorschichte in widersprüchliche Varianten auf, die allerdings anders als in dem filmhistorisch bedeutenden Vorbild nur von einem Erzähler vorgetragen werden: dem namenslosen Helden und Attentäter höchstselbst. Die einzelnen Geschichten unterscheiden sich nicht nur im Inhalt, sondern vor allem auch in den Farben der Mise en Scène, der Kostüme und des Lichts, das auf die Figuren fällt.

Zhang Yimous Hero handelt von der schillernden Wahrheit, die sich dem Zugriff des Menschen prinzipiell entzieht, und konfrontiert ein globales Publikum zugleich mit den emotionalen Wirkungen fast monochrom leuchtender Farbflächen. Die bunten Farben der einzelnen Sequenzen scheinen sich in dem schwarzen Gewand des namenlosen Helden wie in der subtraktiven Farbmischung zu überlagern, bis sie ausgelöscht sind. Viele Zuordnungen, zum Beispiel die glühend roten Inszenierungen der Eifersuchtsszenen sind international unmittelbar zu verstehen. Zugleich ist die Farbensprache des Films Hero zutiefst in der chinesischen Kultur verankert. Sie verweist auf das traditionelle Farbkonzept der Fünf Wandlungsphasen, das Bestandteil der chinesischen Philosophie des korrelativen Denkens ist. Der Vortrag nähert sich dem Thema der Farbdramaturgie und der kulturspezifischen Symbolsprache der Farben aus filmwissenschaftlicher und sinologischer Perspektive und fächert die konkurrierenden Interpretationsmöglichkeiten von Hero am Beispiel ausgewählter Szenen auf.



Biographie

horres
Robert Horres ist seit 2004 Professor für Japanologie am Asien-Orient-Institut der Universität Tübingen und vertritt dort den Arbeitsbereich Modernes Japan. Er arbeitete an der Forschungsstelle Modernes Japan der Universität Bonn und dem Deutschen Institut für Japanstudien in Tokyo und war Gastwissenschaftler an verschiedenen japanischen Institutionen, wie dem National Institute for Science and Technology Policy und dem historiographischen Institut der Universität Tokyo.

Zu seinen Forschungsschwerpunkten gehören Technik, Gesellschaft und Kultur in Japan. Aktuell arbeitet er an einem Projekt zur Geschichte des Holzhandwerks in Japan. Ein weiteres Projekt ist im Bereich Digital Humanities angesiedelt und befasst sich Problemen der Digitalisierung kulturellen Erbes.

Abstract: Traditionelle Farben Japans

Ausgehend von einer lexikographischen Betrachtung und einer Einführung in die Problematik historischer Farben wird eine Betrachtung des Feldes der traditionellen Farben Japans angestellt.
Dabei werden Symbolik, Verwendungskontexte und soziale Markierung der traditionellen Farbsystematik als Referenzsystem verstanden, welches in diversen modernen Kontexten der japanischen Gesellschaft bis heute wirkmächtig ist.

Einen Schwerpunkt der Ausführungen bilden die mit dem Feld traditionelle und historische Farben verbundenen methodischen und definitorischen Probleme sowie die Frage der Repräsentanz in den Neuen Medien und der heutigen Medienkultur.



Biographie

Yokosawa
Kazuhiko Yokosawa is a Professor of Psychology Department at the University of Tokyo, Japan. He studied information science at Tokyo Institute of Technology, Japan. In 1990, he received a PhD in Engineering from Tokyo Institute of Technology. From 1981-1998 he had worked for NTT Basic Research Laboratory. In 1998 he moved to the University of Tokyo. He studied with Prof. Irving Biederman at University of Southern California from 1995 to 1996 and with Prof. Stephen Palmer at UC Berkeley from 2009 to 2010. He is the former president of Japanese Cognitive Science Society. He is a fellow of the Psychonomic Society and a member of Vision Sciences Society.
His research interests lie in attention, object recognition, and multi-sensory perception. Recently he is concerned with color preference and Japanese grapheme-color synesthesia.

Abstract: Ecological Effects in Cross-Cultural Differences on Single Colour Preferences: The Effect of Symbolic / Conceptual Associations

Palmer and Schloss (2010) reported that preference for a given color is largely determined by affective responses to all of the objects associated with that color. Their ecological valence theory posits that it would be adaptive for people to engage with objects whose colors they like and to avoid objects whose colors they dislike to the extent that their color preferences are correlated with objects that are beneficial/harmful to them. Palmer and Schloss developed an empirical procedure to measure the WAVEs (weighted affective valence estimates) of colors. Each color’s WAVE is the average valence (liking/disliking) rating for all object descriptions given as associations of that color, with each object-valence rating weighted by how well it matches the color for which it was described. U.S. WAVEs strongly predicted average U.S. color preferences (r = .89). For example, people generally like blues and cyans partly because they like blue and cyan objects, such as clear sky and clean water, whereas they generally dislike greenish-browns partly because they dislike greenish-brown objects, such as biological wastes and rotting food.

Color preferences are influenced by color-related experiences within one’s lifetime. Such experiences include a multitude of culturally specific experiences with ecological objects. For example, ecological factors might influence differences between U.S. and Japanese color preferences. Yokosawa, Schloss, Asano, & Palmer (in press) reported that Japanese and U.S. color preferences have both similarities (e.g., peaks around blue, troughs around dark-yellow, and preferences for saturated colors) and differences (e.g., Japanese participants like darker colors less than U.S. participants do). The ecological valence theory implies that within-culture WAVE-preference correlations should be higher than between-culture WAVE-preference correlations. The results supported these predictions. However, object-based WAVEs in Japan predict Japanese color preferences somewhat less strongly (r = .61) than object-based U.S. WAVEs predict U.S. color preferences (r = .89).

Why might object-based WAVEs explain so much less variance in Japan than in the U.S.? Following Palmer, Schloss, Guo, Wung, Chai & Peng (submitted), we hypothesized that symbolic/conceptual associations with colors may also contribute to Japanese color preferences. Symbolic WAVEs were calculated using the same procedure as for object-based WAVEs, except restricting people’s color associations to symbols and abstract concepts. The correlation between color preferences and the symbolic WAVEs in Japan (r = .57) was comparable to that between color preferences and object-based WAVEs (r = .61). Including both symbolic and object WAVEs produced a multiple-R of .72, indicating that both object and symbolic/conceptual associations with colors contribute to single color preferences in Japan. Further analyses show that symbolic WAVEs were as highly correlated with color preferences as object-based WAVEs were in Japan, but not in the U.S. (r = .58 for symbols; r = .89 for objects). Object and symbolic/conceptual associations with colors thus have comparable impact on Japanese single color preferences. This suggests that, unlike in the US where object associations have a dominant role in color preferences, object and symbolic associations contribute equally to color preferences in Japan.



Biographie

yoko
Dr. Yoko Mizokami is an Associate Professor in the Division of Information Science, Graduate School of Advanced Integration Science, Chiba University, Japan.
She received a Ph.D. in Engineering in 2002 from Ritsumeikan University, Japan. From 2002-2006 she was a postdoctoral fellow at the University of Nevada, Reno, Department of Psychology. She moved to Chiba University in 2006.
Her research interests lie in color vision, color science, and vision in natural environments. The current research topics cover the influence of lighting to object appearance, the appearance of skin color and texture, the perception of image color difference, colorfulness adaptation, and vision deficiency.
She received the Konica Minolta Imaging Science Encouragement Award, 2015, the Indow Taro Award, 2010, the Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan, 2000 and the Optical Society of Japan Award, 1999. She is a member of International Colour Vision Society (ICVS), Vision Sciences Society, OSA, Vision Society of Japan, Color Science Association of Japan, Optical Society of Japan, and the Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan.

Abstract: Influence of Diffusibility of Illumination on the Appearance of Colour and Surface Properties

The appearance of object surface could be largely influenced by lighting conditions such as color, direction and diffuseness. The diffuseness of illumination would change the components of specular and diffuse reflection resulting difference in the perceived quality of object surface. However, it has not been systematically analyzed how color and surface appearance of an object is influenced by the diffuseness.

We investigated how the impression of surface appearance of test samples changes under diffused light and direct light using real samples in real miniature rooms. We prepared plane gray test samples with three different levels of surface roughness and gray spheres with matt and gloss surface. A sample was placed in the center of a miniature room with either diffuse or directed light, and observer evaluated its appearance. We used a semantic differential method to examine what types of factors were influenced by the diffusibility of illumination. The result of analysis based on 20 adjective-pairs showed that glossiness and smoothness were main factors. Samples tended to appear less glossy and smother under diffused light than under direct light, and their difference was larger for a sample with rough surface.

We also examined the color appearance of several color samples under diffused light and direct light by selecting corresponding color from a Munsell color chart placed in a separate viewing box. The results of corresponding color were similar in both lighting conditions, suggesting color appearance was stable at least for samples that we tested.

Our results suggest that the appearance of surface qualities such as glossiness and smoothness are more influenced by the diffusibility of illumination than color appearance. It also implies that the surface properties of objects should be considered when examining the influence of diffusibility of illumination on surface appearance.



Biographie

Professor of Neuroethology, SOKENDAI. PhD. Graduated from Jiyu-Gakuen college (natural science course) and Sophia University graduate school (behavioral biology). As the 1st year graduate student, I found butterflies detect light by their genitalis, and analysed the mechanism and function of this unique photoreceptive system for my PhD study. After being a biology professor at Yokohama City University, I moved to SOKENDAI in 2006. Visiting fellow at Australian National University (neurobiology); research student at Mitsubishi-Kasei Institute of Life Science; research fellow at NIH (visual science); researcher of JST-PRESTO, etc.

Abstract: How Do Butterflies See Flowers? Their Eyes and Colour Vision

Wie sehen Schmetterlinge Blumen? Ihre Augen und ihr Farbsehen

Flower-visiting butterflies have color vision, including some sophisticated aspects such as color constancy and simultaneous color contrast. Unlike the trichromatic retinas of humans (blue, green and red cones (plus rods)) and honeybees (UV, blue and green cells), we have found that the compound eyes of butterflies contain six, nine or even fifteen photoreceptor classes in species-specific manner. The eyes of the Japanese yellow swallowtail, Papilio xuthus, contain six classes (UV, violet, blue, green, red and broad-band) of receptors. The six classes of receptors are embedded in small units called ommatidia, each housing nine photoreceptor cells, in three fixed combinations; the Papilio eye is a patchwork of spectrally-heterogeneous ommatidia. For example, one type of ommatidia contains the UV, blue, green and red receptors, together forming a single light-guiding rhabdom. Such organization of rhabdom well explains why Papilio butterflies can discriminate colors of dots whose size correspond to their single pixel. At the molecular level, the Papilio retina expresses five visual pigment opsins. There is therefore no one-to-one relationship between the visual pigments and photoreceptor spectral sensitivities. UV and violet receptors actually share an UV-absorbing visual pigment, while the broad-band receptors coexpress a green-absorbing and a red-absorbing visual pigments, which violates the one receptor-one opsin dogma in vision science. The six classes of receptors are embedded in small units called ommatidia, each housing nine photoreceptor cells, in three fixed combinations; the Papilio eye is a patchwork of spectrally-heterogeneous ommatidia. For example, one type of ommatidia contains the UV, blue, green and red receptors, together forming a single light-guiding rhabdom. Such organization of rhabdom well explains why Papilio butterflies can discriminate colors of dots whose size correspond to their single pixel. At the molecular level, the Papilio retina expresses five visual pigment opsins. There is therefore no one-to-one relationship between the visual pigments and photoreceptor spectral sensitivities. UV and violet receptors actually share an UV-absorbing visual pigment, while the broad-band receptors coexpress a green-absorbing and a red-absorbing visual pigments, which violates the one receptor-one opsin dogma in vision science. Do Papilio use all six for seeing colors, i.e., is Papilio hexachromatic? Trichromatic systems have their best wavelength discrimination ability in two wavelength regions. We found foraging Papilio can discriminate about 1 nm difference in three wavelength regions, which appear even better than in humans. Analysis of the behavioral data using the receptor-noise limited color opponency model indicates that the Papilio color vision is tetrachromatic based on UV, blue, green and red inputs.



Biographie

Andreas Bartels
Andreas Bartels studierte in Zürich und San Diego computational neurobiology. Er doktorierte bei Semir Zeki In London in visuellen Neurowissenschaften wo er neue Methoden entwickelte, die den Gebrauch von natürlichen Filmsequenzen als Stimulus für die Hirnforschung pionierten. Nach einem post-doc am Max Planck Institut in Tübingen leitet er das Vision and Cognition Lab am Centrum für Integrative Neurowissenschaften in Tübingen.

Abstract: Colour in the Cortex

Colours are an illusion created by the brain: our colour percept does not represent the light reaching the eye from a given surface, but instead it represents the brain's estimate of that surface's reflectance. Colours hence appear constant even if the illumination and therefore the reflected light changes - this is referred to as colour constancy. The neuroscience of colour is hence also the science of conscious perception. I will review experimental evidence primarily from neuroimaging that sheds light on neural processing of colour in the human brain: Is there a binding problem between colour and motion?
How do memories of object colours influence neural processing? Do colours in Mondrians activate the same circuits as colours in movies? What are neural effects of paying attention to colour? Where does the brain represent information of the (changing) illuminant and where of (constant) surface reflectance, i.e. colour? Apart from the above I will briefly introduce key technologies such as brain imaging and multi-variate pattern analysis and the concept of predictive coding.



Biographie

Kristin Jandke
Kristin Jandke wurde 1971 in Würzburg geboren. Nach ihrem Textil-Design Studium arbeitete sie in der Color&Trim-Abteilung der smart GmbH an Kreativ- und Wertanmutungs-Konzepten für die Fahrzeugentwicklung.Ihr Verantwortungsbereich umfasste den Einsatz von Lack- und Kunststoff-Effekten im Sinne eines wirtschaftlich sinnvollen Einsatzes sowie die Kreation von hochwertigen, belastbaren Kunststoff-Oberflächen, Texturen und Narbungen.
Im weiteren Verlauf war sie als freiberufliche Designerin und Künstlerin tätig, unter anderem mit Ausstellungen in ihrer Heimatstadt Würzburg.
Aktuell ist Kristin Jandke für die Designagentur B612 in Stuttgart tätig. Hauptaufgaben sind maßgeblich die Ermittlung und Analyse von Trends sowie deren Überleitung in Farb- und Materialkonzepte sowie Kollektionen. Der Kreis der Kunden erstreckt sich von der Automobilindustrie bis hin zur Sportartikel-Branche.

Abstract: Natur, Natürlich- Trend, Interpretation und Umsetzung. Wie ein Naturtrend entsteht, wie er auf einem Produkt umgesetzt wird

Der von Kristin Jandke angebotene Workshop soll die Veränderung des Verhältnisses von Mensch zur Natur beleuchten, die Auswirkung auf die jährlichen Trendströmungen und die Potenziale dieser unerschöpflichen Inspirationsquelle aufzeigen. Dabei stellt die Umsetzung der Farben ein ständige Herausforderung dar – „natürliche“ Farben auf einem „künstlichen“ Produkt. Wie können artifizielle Produkte, wie Lacke auf Fahrzeugen, Textilfarben auf Bekleidung, Fassaden und Flächen eine natürliche Anmutung erhalten, ohne natürlich zu sein – Natur: Gegenpol zum künstlich Geschaffenen...!?



Nächste Seite »